The Vervet Monkey is native to much of Southern and East Africa,
being found from Ethiopia, Somalia and extreme southern South Sudan,
to South Africa. They inhabit savanna, riverine woodland, coastal
forest, and mountains up to 4000 m. They are adaptable and able
to persist in secondary and/or highly fragmented vegetation, including
cultivated areas, and sometimes are found living in both rural and
urban environments. They eat a primarily herbivorous diet, and live
mostly on wild fruits, flowers, leaves, seeds, and seed pods. They
may also take advantage of bean, pea, young tobacco, vegetable,
fruit, and grain crops and animals such as grasshoppers, termites
or eggs and chicks. They have black faces and grey body hair color,
ranging in body length from about 40 cm for females who weigh between
3.4 and 5.3 kg, to about 50 cm for males who average a weight of
5.5 kg. The species exhibits sexual dimorphism; the males are larger
in weight and body length and may be recognized by a turquoise-blue
scrotum. When males reach sexual maturity, they move to a neighboring
group while females remain in their groups throughout life. Separate
dominance hierarchies are found for each sex. Male hierarchies are
determined by age, tenure in the group, fighting abilities, and
allies, while female hierarchies are dependent on maternal social
status. A large proportion of interactions occurs between individuals
that are similarly ranked and closely related. Between unrelated
individuals, female competition exists for grooming members of high-ranking
families, presumably to gain more access to resources. These observations
suggest individual recognition is possible and enables discrimination
of genetic relatedness and social status. Interactions between different
groups are variable, ranging from highly aggressive to friendly.
Furthermore, individuals seem to be able to recognise cross-group
vocalisations, and identify from and to which monkey each call is
intended, even if the call is made by a subadult male, which is
likely to transfer groups. This suggests the members within a group
are actively monitoring the activity of other groups, including
the movement of individuals within a group. In addition to behavioral
research on natural populations, vervet monkeys serve as a nonhuman
primate model for understanding genetic and social behaviors of
humans. They have been noted for having human-like characteristics,
such as hypertension, anxiety, and social and dependent alcohol
use. Interestingly, a juvenile scream elicits a reaction from all
mothers, yet the juvenile's own mother has a shorter latency in
looking in the direction of the scream, as well as an increased
duration in her look. Further, mothers have been observed to help
their offspring in conflict, yet rarely aid other juveniles. Other
mothers evidently can determine to which mother the offspring belongs.
Individuals have been observed to look towards the mother whose
offspring is creating the scream which suggests a theory of mind.
In groups of vervet monkeys, infants are the target of a tremendous
amount of attention. Days after an infant is born, every member
of the group inspects the infant at least once by touching or sniffing.
While all group members participate in infant caretaking, juvenile
females that cannot yet menstruate are responsible for the majority
of allomothering. Spiteful actions are extremely rare in the animal
kingdom. Often, an indirect benefit is gained by the individual
acting 'spitefully', or by a close relative of that individual.
Vervet monkeys have been observed to destroy a competitor's food
source rather than consume or steal it themselves. While energy
is being lost on destroying the food, an advantage is obtained by
the individual due to an increase in competitive gain. Although
according to the IUCN its conversation status is of "least concern,"
they are used for biomedical research, and many people living in
close proximity to vervet colonies see them as pests, as they steal
their food.
|